Join Us | Latest Articles | Contact

Journal Home


Editorial Board


Archive


Submit to this journal


Current issue

International Archives of Addiction Research and Medicine





DOI: 10.23937/2474-3631/1510011



Cross-Cultural Research on Internet Addiction: A Systematic Review

Olatz Lopez-Fernandez*


Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Universite Catholique de Louvain, Belgium


*Corresponding author: Olatz Lopez-Fernandez, Laboratory for Experimental Psychopathology, Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Universite Catholique de Louvain 10, Place du Cardinal Mercier - 1348 Louvain-La-Neuve, Belgium, Tel: 320-10 4-72250, Fax: 320-104-73774, E-mail: olatz.lopez@uclouvain.be; lopez.olatz@gmail.com
Int Arch Addict Res Med, IAARM-1-011, (Volume 1, Issue 2), Research Article; ISSN: 2474-3631
Received: September 22, 2015 | Accepted: October 27, 2015 | Published: October 29, 2015
Citation: Lopez-Fernandez O (2015) Cross-Cultural Research on Internet Addiction: A Systematic Review. Int Arch Addict Res Med 1:011. 10.23937/2474-3631/1510011
Copyright: © 2015 Lopez-Fernandez O. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.



Abstract

Internet addiction (IA) is the most studied behavioral addiction. However, few studies have been conducted that simultaneously include several countries to attain an inter-cultural and international perspective on the phenomenon. The aim of this systematic review is to observe the characteristics of cross-cultural IA studies, in order to explore (i) their classification, (ii) the cultures involved, (iii) the IA issues treated, and (iv) the commonalities and differences among cultures. For this review the PsycINFO scientific database was selected to make searches without restrictions. Out of 23 documents, 8 were selected (34.8%), after applying two research paper selection phases with the following specific inclusion criteria: the paper needs to be (i) an empirical cross-cultural study, (ii) focused on IA or other related online excessive behaviors, and (iii) published in a journal. Between 2008 and 2015, the results show that from 2012 onwards until the present, there has been a continuing increase of papers, mostly cross-national intercontinental studies that usually research IA in its generalized subtype and estimate its prevalence with classic and valid scales, while only one study focuses on video gaming. The continents involved are usually Asia, America and Europe, and the commonalities show that the higher rates of IA exist in Asian countries, in male users who spend a lot of hours online. Differences among countries and risk factors have however also been detected and cultural factors are usually not analyzed. More cross-cultural research on IA and other potentially addictive technologies and online activities is needed, so that we may further explore cultural factors and achieve higher external validity of the findings.


Keywords

Internet addiction, Information and communication technologies, Cross-cultural research, Systematic review


Introduction

Since 1996, internet addiction (IA) has been the most studied behavioral addiction, and it is usually conceptualized as the excessive generalized usage of information and communication technologies (ICT) [1]. Nowadays, IA has gained a privileged status due to the last Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn (DSM-5) of the American Psychiatric Association (APA [2]). In this classification "Internet Gaming Disorder" (IGD) was included in the appendix (section III), in order to be studied for a potential future inclusion in the next DSM. Relating to this matter, Petry and colleagues [3] have recently published a highly debated paper [4], which points out that one of the future research lines in the study of IGD is cross-cultural research.

Cross-cultural research uses information from different social groups in its attempt to obtain a general explanation of a phenomenon [5]. This type of research provides a comparison between different cultures by identifying patterns, as well as by looking for commonalities such as universally shared knowledge. Cross-cultural research has been classified along up to four dimensions, according to the following parameters: (i) geographical scope, (ii) type of data used, (iii) time of data collection, and (iv) the types of sample size [6]. One of the strengths of this type of research is that it aims for external validity, using the comparison for the purpose of generalizability. However, the researcher must be familiar with these societies, due to which it is not unusual to see several authors, at least one of each culture, participating in this type of scientific production.

Previous reviews in IA [7,8] have stated that prevalence rates are problematic because they differ as a consequence of different assessment tools and cut-offs points. Similarly, the lack of a clear conceptualization of IA has produced the application of diverse assessment strategies with inconsistent diagnostic criteria. Cross-cultural research, however, could provide a solution for IA by introducing valid instruments with a common method to estimate prevalence. In this regard, this strategy could bridge part of the existing gap in the literature related to the assessment of IA-related problems through self-report measures, by adapting it to different cultures in order to estimate prevalence and other related measures (e.g., psychological constructs such as personality, impulsivity, etc.).

After two decades of research production in IA, only few cross-cultural studies have been conducted in this field relating to potential technology use disorders. This review intends to explore the existing cross-cultural research in order: (i) to extract the classification that is commonly used in this type of study, (ii) to see which cultures are usually compared, (iii) to learn which IA issues are studied, and (iv) to explore the commonalities and differences between peoples' excessive online behaviors.


Method

Search strategy

The literature review was performed by using the basic Proquest search option in the PsycINFO Database (search conducted in August 2015 at the Catholic University of Louvain, Belgium). PsycINFO was selected because it is the most important scientific search engine in Psychology. The search equation was the following: [((internet addict*) or (problematic internet use) or (pathologic internet use)) AND cross-cultur* NOT drug*].

In the first phase, a total of 30 documents were retrieved: 23 were peer-reviewed results from scholarly journals (the rest were books) and the publication dates ranged from 2003 to 2015. From 2012 onwards, there has been an increasing tendency to publish these kinds of studies, due to which 69.57% of the results are relatively recent (Figure 1). Journal titles with more than one result were: Addiction (n = 3), Computers in human behavior (n = 2), CyberPsychology & Behavior (n = 2), and Personality and Individual Differences (n = 2), all of which have a relevant impact factor according to the Journal Citation Reports. The subjects identified by the database that produced more than two results were: cross-cultural differences (n = 18), IA (n = 10), psychometrics (n = 4), test validity (n = 4), test reliability (n = 3), college students (n = 3), and internet (n = 3). Similarly, the classification categories that produced more than two results were: Behavior Disorders & Antisocial Behavior (n = 8), Substance Abuse & Addiction (n = 4) and Clinical Psychological Testing (n = 3). In relation to methodology, 70% of the described studies were empirical (n = 16) with a preponderance of the quantitative approach (n = 13), and their samples were adults (n = 14) and adolescents (n = 8) from both genders equally. All the documents were in English.


.
Figure 1: PsycINFO search. The bars represent the volume of documents published each year from October 2003 to September 2015. View Figure 1



.




In the second phase, a manual revision was conducted to select from the corpus of 23 documents those meeting the following three criteria: (i) they describe empirical cross-cultural research (i.e., comparing at least two cultures), (ii) they focus on IA (including other key words with a similar meaning; e.g., Problematic Internet Use (PIU) or other related online excessive behaviors (thus removing drugs)), and (iii) they are journal articles (thus removing books). As a result, 8 journal papers describing cross-cultural studies of technology use disorders were selected to be analyzed, constituting 34.8% of the initial sample.


Analytical strategy

An applied thematic (content) analysis was used [9] with the following pre-defined codes (or themes) system based on the research objectives: (i) a cross-cultural classification design study [5,6] including (i.a) the geographical scope (i.e., regions [R], countries [COUNT], and continents [CONT]), (i.b) the type of sources used as data (i.e., primary [P] vs. secondary [S]), (i.c) the type of data collection (i.e., synchronic [S] vs. diachronic [D]), and (i.d) the types of sample size (i.e., study case [SC], small [SS] or larger sample [LS] sizes); (ii) the cultures compared (name of the cultures, e.g., Europe vs. North-America); (iii) the IA issues investigated, and (iv) the commonalities and/or differences between cultures. However, flexibility was taken into account to have the freedom to collect other relevant information not exactly matching the four categories, including the respective subcategories in the case of the first one (classifications), which presumably were exhaustive and mutually exclusive.


Preliminary Results

Characteristics of the cross-cultural studies and its classification

Table 1 summarizes the characteristics of the 8 studies selected with its objective, cross-cultural classification design. In this classification, the emergent data forced the creation of a subcategory inside the "(i.b) type of sources," adding a third code, mixed [M], which was a combination of primary and secondary data sources in the same study that would allow for the cross-cultural comparison, typical in diachronic designs (in which a previous part contains data from the present research, and the second part data from other previous research; or vice-versa). Also mentioned are sample size (including descriptive gender and age socio-demographics), the IA instrument (test) used and the main results (findings).



Table 1: Cross-cultural studies reviewed: characteristics and classification. View Table 1


Almost all the studies were cross-national bridging different continents, due to which they were categorized as continentals [10,12,14,21,23,24]. The remaining studies compared countries on the same continent [16,18]. They all used primary sources collected by surveys using IA scales [14,15,17,18,21], synchronically in all cultures, except one. This exception [12] used mixed sources diachronically, in order to replicate a study previously conducted in Spain to validate a new videogame scale, so that throughout the paper their results could be compared with the original study conducted seven years ago [10]. Concerning sample sizes, almost all studies used small sample sizes (e.g. approximately 200 participants per country), and some had large samples sizes (consisting of approximately 1000 participants [16,24]). No cross-cultural studies were detected that had a geographical scope different from a country (i.e., regions in a same country with different cultures). Similar to the parameter of sources, the prevalent option was to use primary data collected by researchers. As Ember and Ember [5,6] highlight, almost all cross-cultural studies are synchronic. Additionally, no study cases with different sample sizes were detected, with the prevalent option being the small sample sizes of the groups studied.


Cultures compared

The compared cultures were usually located on different continents, and generally the comparison was between two continents, such as America and Asia (i.e., United States (US) vs. China [10], US vs. Taiwan [24], US vs. United Arab Emirate (UAE) [22]) or America and Europe (i.e., US vs. Spain [12], US vs. Germany [14]), or exceptionally more than two (i.e. America, Asia and Europe [21]). Other studies compared cultures co-existing within the same continent with different strategies, such as a multi-group comparison (i.e., Durkee et al. [16] with European countries such as Austria, Estonia, France, Germany, Hungary, Ireland, Israel, Italy, Romania, Slovenia, Spain and Sweden) or made a two-group comparison (i.e. Japan vs. China [17]), which is the simplest form of comparing cultures in an explorative way).


IA issues

The most studied issue was IA in its generalized subtype, using the classical international instruments to estimate its prevalence such as the Young Diagnostic Questionnaire (YDQ) [17], the Internet Addiction Test (IAT) [19,20], and the Compulsive Internet Use Scale (CIUS) [15], among other ad-hoc items related to this type of technological addiction. Only one study focused on video gaming, using the Problem Video game Playing (PVP) Questionnaire [13].


Commonalities vs. differences

In general, studies of Eastern populations found more IA than the Western ones (e.g., in China there were 14% heavy Internet addicted students vs. 4% in their US counterparts, noting that the Chinese study related the hours online as well as the frequency to connect to IA, while in the American study only the hours counted [10]). If we include intra-continental Eastern cross-cultural studies, higher rates of IA can be observed that show significant differences (e.g., 18.4% in Japanese studies vs. 10.2% in Chinese studies [18]).

In the Western continents, such as Europe, 4% of adolescent internet users have been found to be problematic [16], which is associated with males, and also with hours online [21]. However, on this continent differences in relation to IA exist between countries. In Estonia and Slovenia and above all Israel (which was included in this European study) IA rates are higher, all of which are countries that are located near to the Asian continent. This evidence is also showed by inter-continental cross-cultural studies, in which China and Taiwan (together with Germany) are the countries with a higher prevalence of IA.

Furthermore, in the Eastern adult samples, a recent study [22] has shown that the UAE held significantly higher collectivist values than a US sample, but they continue to obtain higher IA measures. However, it should be pointed out that we still know less about the vulnerabilities in the Eastern or highly collectivistic cultures, and these factors should be investigated in relation to IA.


Discussion

The analyzed cross-cultural studies in IA date from 2008 until the present, which is less than a decade. The classification most commonly used in this type of studies was extracted: usually they are continental and trans-national, they have primary data as sources, they are synchronous through cross-sectional studies, and they have little sample sizes per cultural group studied.

The cultures most frequently compared usually involve three continents (ordered by higher to lower frequency): Asia, America and Europe. It is possible that this interest is due to the higher prevalence rates in Asian countries, and it is remarkable that no studies have been conducted (or published) in Oceania or Africa, which are continents that have a high IT usage as well. However, a large multi-national study conducted in Europe confirms approximately 4% of IA in adolescents with the YDQ, finding differences among the 11 EU countries [16]. However, a similar study conducted in Europe by Tsitsika and colleagues (i.e., COUNT/P/S/LS [26]) only found approximately 1% of adolescents with IA in seven countries (i.e., Greece, Spain, Poland, Germany, Romania, the Netherlands, and Iceland [26]) with the IAT with variation among countries. Another recent cross-cultural study that involved 25 European countries and used an ad-hoc measure, found an average of 2.6% of excessive internet use (EIU [27]), with differences among countries as well (e.g., high EIU in subjects with communication difficulties in Bulgaria and the lowest for risky online behavior in Italy). Therefore, the problem detected in the epidemiologic IA studies is observed once again, with different prevalence rates due to different tools [7]. However, it seems that the prevalence estimated in cross-cultural studies is lower than in the non-cross-cultural studies, with a narrow rate of IA (i.e., from 1 to 4.4%). Considering the capacity of generalization of these studies, it is feasible to consider these data more externally valid, due to the fact of usually replicate simultaneously (or diachronically) the same research strategy in different contexts in order to obtain probably similar findings (i.e., ecological validity).

Another aspect of this study is the nature of the IA issues discussed in the current cross-cultural studies. It is clear that all focus on generalized IA, except one which focuses on video gaming [12]. A few other studies have however also been conducted in an attempt to approach the recent phenomenon of the IGD [28]. With the advent of IGD and proposals such as the one made by Petry and colleagues [3], it is quite probable that soon a cross-cultural study will appear that focuses on this new phenomenon. However, it would also be interesting to learn from other cross-cultural research studies of other technological addictions, such as problematic mobile phone use, or other excessive online behaviors (e.g., such as cybersex, online gambling, etc.).

In relation to the commonalities in cross-cultural researches it seems that usually the most common strategy is the comparison of nations through classical IA tools, such as Young [17,20] scales, in order to estimate the phenomenon's prevalence (e.g., Eastern vs. Western countries, with the first ones always having higher rates of IA, in males and in those who spend a lot of hours online). However, the most differences have been detected in the prevalence estimated in intra-continental countries in combination with some detected risk factors (e.g., hours or frequency online), but almost no cultural factors have been studied (e.g., only one cross-cultural research addressed this explicitly [22]).

More cross-cultural research is needed in IA as generalized and specialized subtypes, per technology, per online activity or behavior that could include addictive components, addressing protective and risks factors, as well as cultural factors. It would especially be interesting to have more descriptive studies conducted in the countries and continents that remain under-represented until the present (e.g., Oceania), as well as relational studies (e.g., associating psychological mechanisms with online excessive usages, or predicting potential IA). A larger challenge could be provided by the replication of causal experiments in different cultures, as well as in-depth qualitative investigations into the phenomenology of these potentially addictive behaviors cross-culturally. The results of this challenging research may provide internal and external validity, as well as a holistic scenario concerning the extent and nature of this complex phenomenon: the potential technological behavioral addictions from a world-wide approach.


Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a grant from the European Commission ("Tech Use Disorders"; Grant ID: FP7-PEOPLE-2013-IEF-627999) awarded to Olatz Lopez-Fernandez.


References
  1. Caplan SE (2010) Theory and measurement of generalized problematic internet use: a two-step approach. Computers in Human Behavior 26: 1089-1097.

  2. American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th (edn) Arlington, VA, American Psychiatric Association.

  3. Petry NM, Rehbein F, Gentile DA, Lemmens JS, Rumpf H, et al. (2014) An international consensus for assessing internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach. Addiction, 109: 1399-1406.

  4. Mark D Griffiths, Tony van Rooij UGent, Daniel Kardefelt-Winther, Vladan Starcevic, Orsolya Kiraly, et al. (2015) Working towards an international consensus on criteria for assessing Internet Gaming Disorder: A critical commentary on Petry et al (2014). Addiction.

  5. Ember CR, Ember M (1998) On cross-cultural research. In: H. Russell Bernard Clarence C. Gravlee, Handbook of methods in cultural anthropology. (1st edn), Altamira Press, Lanham, 647-687.

  6. Ember CR (2009) Cross-cultural research methods (2nd edition). Lanham, Altamira Press.

  7. Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD, Karila L, Billieux J (2014) Internet addiction: A systematic review of epidemiological research for the last decade. Curr Pharm Des 20: 4026-4052.

  8. Lopez-Fernandez O (2015) How has Internet addiction research evolved since the advent of Internet gaming disorder? An overview of cyberaddictions from a psychological perspective. Current Addiction Reports 2: 263-271.

  9. Guest G (2012) Applied thematic analysis. Thousand Oaks, Sage.

  10. Zhang L, Amos C, McDowell WC (2008) A comparative study of internet addiction between the United States and china. CyberPsychology & Behavior 11: 725-729.

  11. Wang W (2001) Internet dependency and psychosocial maturityamong college students.International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 55: 919-938.

  12. Hart GM, Johnson B, Stamm B, Angers N, Robinson A, et al. (2009) Effects of video games on adolescents and adults. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 12: 63-65.

  13. Salguero RAT, Moran RMB (2002) Measuring problem video gameplaying in adolescents. Addiction 97: 1601-1606.

  14. Karl K, Peluchette J, Schlaegel C (2010) Who's posting facebook faux pas? A cross-cultural examination of personality differences. International Journal of Selection and Assessment 18: 174-186.

  15. Meerkerk G, van den Eijnden RJJM, Franken IHA, Garretsen HFL (2010).Is compulsive Internet use related to sensitivity to reward and punishment, andimpulsivity? Computers in Human Behavior 26: 729-735.

  16. Durkee T, Kaess M, Carli V, Parzer P, Wasserman C, et al. (2012) Prevalence of pathological internet use among adolescents in europe: Demographic and social factors. Addiction 107: 2210-2222.

  17. Young K (1998a) Internet addiction: the emergence of a new clinicaldisorder. CyberPsychol Behav 1: 237-244.

  18. Yang CY, Sato T, Yamawaki N, Miyata M (2013) Prevalence and risk factors of problematic internet use: A cross-national comparison of Japanese and Chinese university students. Transcultural Psychiatry 50: 263-279.

  19. Young KS (1996) Caught in the Net: How to recognize the sign of Internet addiction andwinning strategy for recovery. John Wiley and Sons, New York.

  20. Young KS (1998b) Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clinical disorder. Cyberpsychology & Behavior 1: 237-244.

  21. Sariyska R, Reuter M, Bey K, Sha P, Li M, et al. (2014) Self-esteem, personality and internet addiction: A cross-cultural comparison study. Personality and Individual Differences 61-62: 28-33.

  22. Quinones C, Kakabadse NK (2015) Self-concept clarity, social support, and compulsive internet use: A study of the US and the UAE. Computers in Human Behavior 44: 347-356.

  23. Quinones-Garcia C, Korak-Kakabadse N (2014) Compulsive Internet use inadults: A study of prevalence and drivers within the current economic climatein the UK. Computers in Human Behavior 30: 171-180.

  24. Kononova A, Chiang Y (2015) Why do we multitask with media? predictors of media multitasking among internet users in the united states and taiwan. Computers in Human Behavior 50: 31-41.

  25. Bardhi F, Rohm AJ, Sultan F (2010) Tuning in and tuning out: Mediamultitasking among young consumers. Journal of Consumer Behavior 9: 316-332.

  26. Tsitsika A, Janikian M, Schoenmakers TM, Tzavela EC, Olafsson K, et al. (2014) Internet Addictive Behavior in Adolescence: A Cross-Sectional Study in Seven European Countries. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 17: 528-535.

  27. skarupova K, Olafsson K, Blinka L (2015) Excessive Internet Use and its association with negative experiences:Quasi-validation of a short scale in 25 European countries. Computers in Human Behavior 53: 118-123.

  28. Lopez-Fernandez O, Honrubia-Serrano ML, Baguley T, Griffiths MD (2014) Pathological video game playing in Spanish and British adolescents: Towards the exploration of Internet Gaming Disorder symptomatology. Computers in Human Behavior 41: 304-312.

International Journal of Anesthetics and Anesthesiology (ISSN: 2377-4630)
International Journal of Blood Research and Disorders   (ISSN: 2469-5696)
International Journal of Brain Disorders and Treatment (ISSN: 2469-5866)
International Journal of Cancer and Clinical Research (ISSN: 2378-3419)
International Journal of Clinical Cardiology (ISSN: 2469-5696)
Journal of Clinical Gastroenterology and Treatment (ISSN: 2469-584X)
Clinical Medical Reviews and Case Reports (ISSN: 2378-3656)
Journal of Dermatology Research and Therapy (ISSN: 2469-5750)
International Journal of Diabetes and Clinical Research (ISSN: 2377-3634)
Journal of Family Medicine and Disease Prevention (ISSN: 2469-5793)
Journal of Genetics and Genome Research (ISSN: 2378-3648)
Journal of Geriatric Medicine and Gerontology (ISSN: 2469-5858)
International Journal of Immunology and Immunotherapy (ISSN: 2378-3672)
International Journal of Medical Nano Research (ISSN: 2378-3664)
International Journal of Neurology and Neurotherapy (ISSN: 2378-3001)
International Archives of Nursing and Health Care (ISSN: 2469-5823)
International Journal of Ophthalmology and Clinical Research (ISSN: 2378-346X)
International Journal of Oral and Dental Health (ISSN: 2469-5734)
International Journal of Pathology and Clinical Research (ISSN: 2469-5807)
International Journal of Pediatric Research (ISSN: 2469-5769)
International Journal of Respiratory and Pulmonary Medicine (ISSN: 2378-3516)
Journal of Rheumatic Diseases and Treatment (ISSN: 2469-5726)
International Journal of Sports and Exercise Medicine (ISSN: 2469-5718)
International Journal of Stem Cell Research & Therapy (ISSN: 2469-570X)
International Journal of Surgery Research and Practice (ISSN: 2378-3397)
Trauma Cases and Reviews (ISSN: 2469-5777)
International Archives of Urology and Complications (ISSN: 2469-5742)
International Journal of Virology and AIDS (ISSN: 2469-567X)
More Journals

Contact Us

ClinMed International Library | Science Resource Online LLC
3511 Silverside Road, Suite 105, Wilmington, DE 19810, USA
Email: contact@clinmedlib.org
 

Feedback

Get Email alerts
 
Creative Commons License
Open Access
by ClinMed International Library is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License based on a work at https://clinmedjournals.org/.
Copyright © 2017 ClinMed International Library. All Rights Reserved.