Review Article | OPEN ACCESS DOI: 10.23937/2643-4059/1710043

The Relationship of Sleep Duration, Chronotype, Social Jet Lag and the Risk of Depression: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

Lijuan Gao1,2* and Juan Gao3

1Doctor of Philosophy in Clinical Psychology, Philippines Christian University, 1648 Taft Ave, COR, Metro Manila, 1004, Philippines

2Traditional Chinese Medicine Department, Tianjin Xiqing Hospital, Xiqing District, China

3Associate Professor, The National Police University for Criminal Justice, Lianchi District, China

Abstract

Depression is one of the greatest health challenges worldwide, and sleep is a major influencing factor in depression. This meta-analysis aimed to systematically integrate existing evidence by analyzing 38 observational studies from five electronic databases. Results revealed that short sleep duration (OR=1.78, 95% CI: 1.61-1.97), long sleep duration (OR=1.17, 95% CI: 1.02-1.34), neutral chronotype (N-type) (OR=1.27, 95% CI: 1.06-1.51), evening chronotype (OR=2.09, 95% CI: 1.64-2.68), 1-2 hours of social jet lag (OR=1.11, 95% CI: 1.04-1.19), and ≥2 hours of social jet lag (OR=1.58, 95% CI: 1.4-1.78) were associated with an increased risk of depression. Specifically, short sleep duration, evening chronotype, and ≥2 hours of social jet lag were more significantly associated with the risk of depression. This meta-analysis demonstrated that both short and long sleep durations, evening chronotype, and social jet lag exceeding two hours are associated with an elevated risk of depression. Due to substantial heterogeneity among studies, the evidence base remains limited. Future research should employ standardized methods, longitudinal designs, and investigate contextual factors to clarify underlying mechanisms.

Keywords

Sleep duration, Chronotype, Social jet lag, Depression, Meta-analysis

Introduction

Depression is one of the greatest health challenges worldwide [1]. It affects more than 300 million people globally [2]. According to statistics from the World Health Organization (WHO), depression is one of the leading causes of disability worldwide, and its disease burden is projected to become the highest by 2030 [3]. The treatment and prevention of depression have become a global public health concern. Sleep, as a major influencing factor of depression, has recently attracted increasing attention from scholars.

Our daily lives are regulated by at least three “clocks”: the social clock, the solar clock, and the biological clock [4]. The biological clock is the 24-hour internal time-keeping system, entrained to the environmental light-dark cycle [5]. The human circadian clock controls various physiological process and is particularly noticeable in regulating sleep and wakefulness. Chronotype refers to an individual's preference for the timing of the sleep-wake cycle, also known as circadian preference. Chronotype can be viewed as a biological construct [4]. It is largely regulated by the biological clock. Both genetic variation in clock genes and environmental factors contribute to the distribution of chronotype in the population [6]. Chronotype is typically divided into three categories: morning chronotype, composed of individuals who prefer to go to bed early and rise early; evening chronotype, those who prefer to go to bed late and wake up late; and neutral chronotype [7].

In modern society, the social schedule often conflicts with circadian preferences.

Since the social clock has not adapted to the significant delays observed in most biological clocks, the discrepancy between them has increased substantially [4]. Researchers have proposed the concept of social jet lag [6] which describes and quantifies the chronic mismatch between an individual’s biological clock and the social clock. Typically, social jet lag is more pronounced in evening chronotypes, as morning chronotypes are often forced to stay up late due to social norms. Consequently, neutral chronotypes exhibit the lowest levels of social jet lag [4]. Social jet lag is usually measured as the absolute difference between the midpoint of sleep on free days (MSF) and that on workdays (MSW) [4]. In contemporary society, most individuals, particularly those with evening chronotypes, experience social jet lag. They are required to wake up relatively early on weekdays due to work or school commitments, leading to sleep deprivation. As a result, they attempt to compensate for accumulated sleep debt on weekends or leisure days [4]. Nearly two-thirds of adults in Europe experience at least one hour of social jet lag [8], while approximately 22.95% of adults in Southern China experience more than one hour of social jet lag [9].

Sleep alterations are common in patients with depression. Both long and short sleep durations are frequently observed in individuals with depression. Previous research has predominantly identified short sleep duration as a risk factor for depressive symptoms. However, recent studies have demonstrated that both long and short sleep durations can serve as risk factors for depression [10,11].

Circadian rhythm disturbances are common in patients with depression. In recent years, research on the relationship between chronotype, social jet lag, and depression has garnered significant attention. Individuals with evening chronotypes are at an increased risk for emotional problems, including depressive symptoms. Studies have found that circadian preference is associated with sleep and mental health outcomes in a dose-response manner [12]. Additionally, evening chronotype has been linked to depressive symptoms [13]. Social jet lag has also been positively associated with an elevated risk of depressive symptoms [9,14]. However, a study conducted in the Netherlands found no significant difference in social jet lag between individuals with major depressive disorder and healthy controls [15].

Although the associations between sleep duration, chronotype, social jet lag, and depression have been studied, the findings have been inconsistent. This systematic review and meta-analysis aims to synthesize the relationships between sleep duration, chronotype, social jet lag, and the risk of depression, thereby providing guidelines for the prevention and treatment of depression.

Methods

This systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted in accordance with the Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) checklist [16] and the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta- Analyses (PRISMA) statement [17]. The study was registered in the International Prospective Register of Ongoing Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO; CRD4202455876).

Database searching

We systematically searched the following five databases from their inception to June 2024: PubMed, Web of Science, Embase, PsycINFO, Scopus. The search strategy included the following terms: (sleep duration or chronotype or diurnal preference or circadian rhythm or circadian preference or social jet lag or social jet- lag or social jetlag) and (depression or depressive symptom* or depressive disorder or depressive syndrome* or depression emotional).

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Studies were eligible if they fulfilled the following criteria: (1) Population: Participants were relatively healthy individuals of any age. Studies involving shift workers were excluded. (2) Exposure: Sleep duration was divided into at least three groups (e.g., long sleep group, short sleep group, normal sleep group), studies where sleep duration was divided into two or no groups were excluded; Chronotype was divided into at least three groups (e.g., morning chronotype, evening chronotype, neutral chronotype), studies where chronotype divided into two or no groups were excluded; Social jet lag was divided into at least three groups (e.g., <1 hour, 1-2 hours, ≥2 hours), studies where social jet lag divided into two or no groups were excluded. (3) Outcome: Studies using depressive symptoms as primary or secondary outcome were included. (4) Study design: The included studies were prospective cohort study or cross-sectional studies. (5) Statistical data: Studies were included if they provided adjusted or unadjusted odds ratios (ORs), hazard ratios (HRs), or relative risks (RRs) and their corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs) or sufficient data to calculate them.

Data collection

A data collection form was created by the first author and reviewed by the second author. Two authors independently extracted data from the included articles and recorded this information into the collection form. The following information from the included studies was collected: the first author, year of publication, country, study population, sample size, study design, participants’ average age or age range, participants’ gender, sleep indicators and their measurement methods, depression measurement methods, effect estimates, and their corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs).

Study quality assessment

Two authors independently evaluated the methodological quality of the included prospective cohort studies using the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS). For the included cross-sectional studies, the methodological quality was assessed using a quality assessment tool for analytical cross-sectional studies consisting of eight items. Two authors made judgments of “yes,” “no,” “unclear,” or “inadaptation” for each evaluation item respectively and determined whether to include or exclude the study after discussion. The NOS comprises eight items, divided into three categories: selection, comparability, and outcome. Study quality scores ranged from zero to nine. Studies with scores above six were classified as high quality, scores of five and six were classified as medium quality, and scores below five were classified as low quality.

Meta-analysis

Meta-analysis was performed using Stata/MP version 18.0. A random-effects meta-analysis model was adopted to investigate the relationships of sleep duration, chronotype, and social jet lag with the risk of depression separately. The odds ratio (OR) was used as the effect estimate to assess the association between sleep duration, chronotype, social jet lag, and depression. Analyses of adjusted or unadjusted ORs, hazard ratios (HRs), or relative risks (RRs) and their corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were conducted separately. When available, multivariable-adjusted ORs were preferentially pooled. If adjusted estimates were unavailable, unadjusted estimates were pooled. In sensitivity analysis, the leave-one-out method was applied to examine the influence of individual studies on the pooled ORs. Heterogeneity was quantified using I²statistics, with values less than 25%, between 25% and 50%, and greater than 75% representing low, moderate, and high levels of heterogeneity, respectively. Subgroup analysis and meta-regression analysis were performed to explore potential sources of heterogeneity. Publication bias was evaluated using funnel plots and Egger’s tests.

Results

Literature search

A total of 2,994 records were identified through database searches, with 833 from PubMed, 368 from Web of Science, 814 from Embase, 175 from PsycINFO, and 371 from Scopus. After removing duplicate records, 1,415 records remained. Following the screening of titles and abstracts, 94 full-text articles were retrieved. After reviewing the full-text articles, 11 review articles, 17 irrelevant studies, and 21 studies that did not provide sufficient data to calculate risk estimates were excluded. Additionally, sleep indicators in four studies were divided into two groups, the subjects of two studies were shift workers, and one study was a pilot study. Finally, 38 studies were included in the meta-analysis: four longitudinal studies on sleep duration, 16 cross-sectional studies on sleep duration, 14 cross-sectional studies on chronotype, and four cross-sectional studies on social jet lag. Figure 1 illustrates the flowchart of the article selection process.

Figure 1: PRISMA flowchart of literature search. View Figure 1

Study characteristics

The characteristics of the included studies were displayed in Table 1. The meta- analysis included 755,646 participants. Sample sizes ranged from 200 [18] to 225,915 (Um et al., 2023) [19]. Four longitudinal studies [19-22] included 254,271 participants, sixteen cross-sectional studies [23-38] included 227,028 participants, fourteen cross-sectional studies on chronotype [12,18,39-50] included 233,761 participants, four cross-sectional studies on social jet lag [9,51-53] included 40,586 participants. Among the included studies, four longitudinal studies on sleep duration were conducted in Asia, while four cross-sectional studies on social jet lag were also conducted in Asia. Among sixteen cross-sectional studies on sleep duration, twelve were conducted in Asia, two in Europe, and two in North America. Among fourteen cross-sectional studies on chronotype, nine were conducted in Asia, four in Europe, and one in South America. All studies measured sleep duration, chronotype, and social jet lag using self-report questionnaires or scales, and depression was assessed using the same method.

Table 1: Characteristics of included studies in the meta-analysis. View Table 1

Study quality

According to the NOS, among the included longitudinal studies, three were of moderate quality, and one was of weak quality. The quality scores of the included cross-sectional studies are presented in Table 2.

Table 2: Quality assessment of included cross-sectional studies. View Table 2

Data synthesis

Longitudinal study results

The longitudinal study results showed that short sleep duration was associated with an increased risk of depression compared to the normal sleep group (OR=1.24, 95% CI: 1.06-1.46, I²= 62.1%). However, long sleep duration was not found to increase the risk of depression (OR=0.92, 95% CI: 0.79-1.08, I²= 84.6%).

Cross-Sectional study results (sleep duration)

As shown in Figure 2, the cross-sectional study results indicated that both short sleep duration (OR=1.78, 95% CI: 1.61-1.97, I²= 94.0%) and long sleep duration (OR=1.17, 95% CI: 1.02-1.34, I²=90.9%) were associated with an increased risk of depression compared to the normal sleep group. Notably, short sleep duration was more significantly associated with the risk of depression.

Figure 2: Forest plot of cross-sectional studies on sleep duration. View Figure 2

Cross-Sectional study results (chronotype)

As shown in Figure 3, the cross-sectional study results revealed that both neutral chronotype (N-type) (OR=1.27, 95% CI: 1.06-1.51, I²=84.1%) and evening chronotype (E-type) (OR=2.09, 95% CI: 1.64-2.68, I²= 93.3%) were associated with an increased risk of depression compared to morning chronotype (M-type). Furthermore, evening chronotype was more significantly associated with the risk of depression.

Figure 3: Forest plot of cross-sectional studies on chronotype. View Figure 3

Cross-Sectional study results (Social Jet Lag)

 The cross-sectional study results demonstrated that both 1-2 hours of social jet lag (OR=1.11, 95% CI: 1.04-1.19, I²= 0) and ≥2 hours of social jet lag (OR=1.58, 95% CI: 1.4-1.78, I²=18.5%) were associated with an increased risk of depression compared to the <1 hour social jet lag group. Moreover, ≥2 hours of social jet lag was more significantly associated with the risk of depression.

Subgroup analyses

Subgroup analyses of sleep duration and risk of depression

In subgroup analyses of sleep duration and the risk of depression, the pooled ORs slightly varied by geographic location, participants’ mean age, gender, and sample size. Geographic location may be a source of heterogeneity for both short sleep duration and long sleep duration, while age may be a source of heterogeneity only for long sleep duration (p < 0.05).

Subgroup analyses of chronotype and risk of depression

In subgroup analyses of chronotype and the risk of depression, the pooled ORs slightly varied by geographic location, participants’ mean age, gender, and sample size. However, no significant sources of heterogeneity were identified (p > 0.05).

Meta-regression analysis

Meta-Regression analysis of sleep duration and risk of depression

In the meta-regression analysis of sleep duration and the risk of depression, sample size, participants’ mean age, gender, and geographic location were included as covariates. However, no significant sources of heterogeneity were identified.

Meta-Regression analysis of chronotype and risk of depression

In the meta-regression analysis of chronotype and the risk of depression, sample size, participants’ mean age, gender, and geographic location were included as covariates. However, no significant sources of heterogeneity were identified.

Sensitivity analysis

In the sensitivity analysis, the leave-one-out method was adopted to examine the influence of individual studies on the pooled odds ratios (ORs). For sleep duration and risk of depression: In cross-sectional studies, the pooled ORs for depression ranged from 1.73 (95% CI: 1.57-1.92) to 1.81 (95% CI: 1.63-2.01) for short sleep duration and from 1.12 (95% CI: 0.99-1.28) to 1.21 (95% CI: 1.08-1.37) for long sleep duration. The results indicated that none of the individual studies had an excessive influence on the pooled effect estimates. For chronotype and risk of depression: The pooled ORs for depression ranged from 1.19 (95% CI: 1.02-1.39) to 1.36 (95% CI: 1.15-1.60) for neutral chronotype and from 1.98 (95% CI: 1.57-2.5) to 2.21 (95% CI: 1.71-2.86) for evening chronotype. The results demonstrated that none of the individual studies overly influenced the pooled effect estimates.

Publication bias

The funnel plots of cross-sectional studies on sleep duration and chronotype showed basic symmetry on both sides (Figure 4, Figure 5). The results of Egger’s test (P = 0.04) indicated slight publication bias for the analysis of the association between short sleep duration and the risk of depression in cross-sectional studies. In addition, the results of Egger’s test showed no evidence of publication bias for the analyses of short sleep duration in longitudinal studies (P = 0.09), long sleep duration in cross-sectional studies (P = 0.60) and longitudinal studies (P = 0.95), neutral chronotype (P = 0.40), evening chronotype (P = 0.50), 1-2 hours of social jet lag (P = 0.34), or ≥2 hours of social jet lag (P = 0.25) and their respective associations with the risk of depression.

Figure 4: Funnel plot of cross-sectional studies on sleep duration. View Figure 4

Figure 5: Funnel plot of cross-sectional studies on chronotype. View Figure 5

Discussion

This meta-analysis investigated both longitudinal and cross-sectional associations between sleep duration, chronotype, social jet lag, and the risk of depression, highlighting the critical role of sleep-related factors in depression development. However, due to the limited number of longitudinal studies on sleep duration (n=4) and cross-sectional studies on social jet lag (n=4), these findings require further validation to strengthen their reliability.

Sleep duration and the risk of depression

Longitudinal studies demonstrated that short sleep duration was significantly associated with an increased risk of depression (OR=1.24, 95% CI: 1.06-1.46), whereas \long sleep duration did not appear to elevate this risk (OR=0.92, 95% CI: 0.79-1.08). These findings suggested that sleep deprivation may have acted as a risk factor for depression, potentially mediated by neuroendocrine dysfunction [54], impaired emotional regulation [55], and cognitive decline [56]. In contrast, cross-sectional studies indicated that both short sleep duration (OR=1.78, 95% CI: 1.61-1.97) and long sleep duration (OR=1.17, 95% CI: 1.02-1.34) were associated with a higher risk of depression. Together, these results suggested a potential U-shaped relationship between sleep duration and depression risk, where deviations from the average sleep duration-both shorter and longer-may have increased vulnerability to depression.

Chronotype and the risk of depression

Cross-sectional studies revealed that both neutral chronotype (N-type) (OR=1.27, 95% CI: 1.06-1.51) and evening chronotype (E-type) (OR=2.09, 95% CI: 1.64-2.68) were significantly associated with an increased risk of depression, with the evening chronotype posing a higher risk. This finding may be related to the greater susceptibility of evening-type individuals to social jet lag. Social jet lag refers to the mismatch between an individual's biological clock and societal schedules. Evening chronotype individuals often find their inherent biological clocks mismatched with the work and sleep schedules of modern society [6]. This misalignment can lead to chronic sleep deprivation and disruptions in circadian rhythms. Furthermore, nighttime exposure to artificial light, particularly the blue light emitted by electronic devices, suppresses the secretion of melatonin, further exacerbating circadian rhythm disturbances [57]. Research has shown that circadian rhythm disruption is closely associated with the onset of depression, placing evening chronotype individuals at a higher risk of mental health issues.

Social jet lag and the risk of depression

Cross-sectional studies further revealed that both 1-2 hours of social jet lag (OR=1.11, 95% CI: 1.04-1.19) and ≥2 hours of social jet lag (OR=1.58, 95% CI: 1.4-1.78) were significantly associated with an increased risk of depression. Moreover, the risk of depression rose significantly as the duration of social jet lag increased. These findings highlight the importance of synchronizing daily schedules with biological rhythms for mental health. Social Jet Lag refers to the discrepancy in sleep timing between workdays and rest days for individuals. This misalignment may disrupt the circadian rhythm system [58], impair neuroendocrine functions (e.g., melatonin and cortisol secretion) [6], and affect the functioning of brain regions associated with emotion (e.g., the amygdala and prefrontal cortex) [59]. Such dysfunction may ultimately lead to an increase in depressive symptoms [60].

A meta-analysis has revealed significant heterogeneity in the relationship between sleep duration, chronotype, and social jetlag with the risk of depression. This heterogeneity may stem from variations in study design, sample characteristics, and research environments. In terms of study design, most studies predominantly employed subjective methods to measure sleep metrics and depressive symptoms. Sleep metrics were assessed through questionnaires such as the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire (MCTQ), self-reported questionnaires, and the Morning and Evening Questionnaire (MEQ). Similarly, the evaluation of depressive symptoms relied on various scales, such as Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9), the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), and the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D). Differences in the scoring criteria and sensitivity of these scales may have influenced the consistency of the results. Meanwhile, variations in sample characteristics further contributed to the diversity of the findings. For instance, the studies encompassed a wide age range from adolescents to adults, with subgroup analyses indicating that prolonged sleep duration was associated with an increased risk of depression only in individuals aged 30 and above, while no such association was observed in those under 30. Additionally, the geographical distribution of the samples was diverse, spanning regions such as Asia, Europe, and North America. Subgroup analyses showed that the association between long sleep duration and depression risk was significant only in North America, while no significant association was found in Asia or Europe. This phenomenon may be related to differences in lifestyle habits, cultural backgrounds, and societal pressures across regions, which could modulate the relationship between social jetlag and depressive symptoms.

This study provided preliminary evidence regarding the relationship between sleep duration, chronotype, and social jet lag with the risk of depression; however, some notable limitations should be acknowledged. Firstly, data on sleep duration, chronotype, and social jet lag were collected based on participants’ self-reported questionnaires or scales, a method that may have been subject to recall bias and measurement bias. Due to the lack of objective measurement tools, the estimation of the true associations between these factors and depression risk may have been affected by misclassification. Therefore, future studies were suggested to prioritize the use of standardized and objective measurement methods (e.g., actigraphy or polysomnography) to assess sleep indicators more accurately. Secondly, the majority of studies included in this meta-analysis were cross-sectional in design, a type of study that could not establish causal relationships between variables. To gain a deeper understanding of these associations, future research was recommended to incorporate more longitudinal designs to verify the causal pathways between sleep indicators and depression risk.

Conclusion

This meta-analysis revealed that sleep duration is associated with an increased risk of depression, with both short and long sleep durations showing elevated risks. Among all chronotypes, the evening chronotype was most strongly linked to a higher likelihood of depression. Additionally, social jet lag was found to increase depression risk, particularly when it exceeded two hours. Although these findings provide preliminary evidence, significant heterogeneity among studies and a limited evidence base suggest caution in interpretation. Future research should prioritize standardized and objective measurement methods, incorporate longitudinal designs, and investigate demographic, environmental, and cultural factors to better understand the mechanisms underlying the relationships between sleep duration, chronotype, social jet lag, and depression risk. Such efforts will inform more effective prevention and intervention strategies for depressive symptoms.

Declaration of interests

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Ethical approval

The methodology we used in this study was primarily based on data from published articles, with secondary analyses of existing clinical study reports. As our work did not involve any form of new data collection and all original data had been published by the original authors and had been treated with appropriate privacy protections, the Ethics Approval Committee determined that the project was not subject to the standard ethics approval process and granted a waiver prior to the commencement of this meta-analysis project, in accordance with relevant ethical guidelines and the agency's requirements. However, in order to ensure the integrity and transparency of our research, we strictly follow the relevant ethical norms of the medical and scientific community regarding data processing and the protection of personal privacy throughout the process.

References

  1. Ferrari AJ, Somerville AJ, Baxter AJ, Norman R, Patten SB, et al. (2013) Global variation in the prevalence and incidence of major depressive disorder: A systematic review of the epidemiological literature. Psychol Med 43: 471-481.
  2. Patel V, Chisholm D, Parikh R, Charlson FJ, Degenhardt L, et al. (2016) Addressing the burden of mental, neurological, and substance use disorders: key messages from Disease Control Priorities, 3rd edition. Lancet 387: 1672- 1685.
  3. Malhi GS, Mann JJ (2018) Depression. Lancet 392: 2299-2312.
  4. Roenneberg T, Pilz LK, Zerbini G, Winnebeck EC (2019) Chronotype and Social Jetlag: A (Self-) Critical Review. Biology 8: 54.
  5. Richardson GS (2005) The human circadian system in normal and disordered sleep. J clin psychiat 66 Suppl 9: 3-9.
  6. Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M, Roenneberg T (2006) Social jetlag: misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol Int 23: 497-509.
  7. Adan A, Archer SN, Hidalgo MP, Di Milia L, Natale V, et al. (2012) Circadian typology: A comprehensive review. Chronobiol Int 29: 1153-1175.
  8. Roenneberg T, Kantermann T, Juda M, Vetter C, Allebrandt KV (2013) Light and the human circadian clock. Handb Exp Pharmacol 2013: 311-331.
  9. Feng J, Meng X, Wang C, Ke P, Tian Q, et al. (2023) Associations of social jetlag with depressive symptoms among adults in Southern China: A cross-sectional study. Chronobiol Int 40: 783-794.
  10. Liu BP, Wang XT, Liu ZZ, Wang ZY, An D, et al. (2020) Depressive symptoms are associated with short and long sleep duration: A longitudinal study of Chinese adolescents. J affect disorders 263: 267-273.
  11. Matsushita M, Koyama A, Ushijima H, Mikami A, Katsumata Y, et al. (2014) Sleep duration and its association with sleepiness and depression in "ronin-sei" preparatory school students. Asian J Psychiatr 9: 61-66.
  12. Kjørstad K, Vedaa Ø, Pallesen S, Saxvig IW, Hysing M, et al. (2022) Circadian preference in young adults: Associations with sleep and mental health outcomes from a national survey of Norwegian university students. Chronobiol Int 39: 1465-1474.
  13. Benasi G, Mehr C, Liao M, Aggarwal B (2024) The role of social support in the relation between chronotype and mental health in a cohort of women from the American Heart Association Go Red for Women Strategically Focused Research Network. Chronobiol Int 41: 447-455.
  14. Islam Z, Hu H, Akter S, Kuwahara K, Kochi T, et al. (2020b) Social jetlag is associated with an increased likelihood of having depressive symptoms among the Japanese working population: the Furukawa Nutrition and Health Study. Sleep 43.
  15. Knapen SE, Riemersma-van der Lek RF, Antypa N, Meesters Y, Penninx B, et al. (2018) Social jetlag and depression status: Results obtained from the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety. Chronobiol Int 35: 1-7.
  16. Stroup DF, Berlin JA, Morton SC, Olkin I, Williamson GD, et al. (2000) Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology - A proposal for reporting. Jama-Journal of the American Medical Association 283: 2008-2012.
  17. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Grp P (2009) Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA Statement. Plos Medicine 6.
  18. Hidalgo MP, Caumo W, Posser M, Coccaro SB, Camozzato AL, et al. (2009) Relationship between depressive mood and chronotype in healthy subjects. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 63: 283-290.
  19. Um YJ, Kim Y, Chang Y, Jung HS, Cho IY, et al. (2023) Association of changes in sleep duration and quality with incidence of depression: A cohort study. J Affect Disord 328: 64-71.
  20. Chen R, Chen Q, Lu G, Zhang M, Zhang M, et al. (2023) Sleep duration and depressive symptoms in Chinese middle-aged and older adults: The moderating effects of grip strength. J Affect Disord 339: 348-354.
  21. Jing R, Xu T, Rong H, Lai X, Fang H (2020) Longitudinal association between sleep duration and depressive symptoms in chinese elderly. Nat Sci Sleep 12: 737-747.
  22. Li C, Ma C, Wu H, Zhao M, Zhu D, et al. (2022) Sleep duration change from childhood to adulthood and adult depressive disorders in the Chinese population. Sleep Med 100: 128-132.
  23. Andishmand Z, Amini A, Naderi F, Garmabi M, Sharifnezhad A, et al. (2023) Is sleep duration associated with depression and anxiety? A Cross-Sectional study on medical students in Iran. Sleep Medicine Research 14: 200-206.
  24. Anujuo K, Stronks K, Snijder MB, Lok A, Jean-Louis G, et al. (2021) Association between Depressed Mood and Sleep Duration among Various Ethnic Groups-The Helius Study. Int J Environ Res Public Health 18: 7134.
  25. Chen X, Wang SB, Li XL, Huang ZH, Tan WY, et al. (2020) Relationship between sleep duration and sociodemographic characteristics, mental health and chronic diseases in individuals aged from 18 to 85 years old in Guangdong province in China: A population-based cross-sectional study. BMC psychiatry 20: 455.
  26. Dong L, Xie Y, Zou X (2022) Association between sleep duration and depression in US adults: A cross-sectional study. J Affect Disord 296: 183-188.
  27. Gao Y, Tang W, Mao D, Chen L, Ding X (2023) Association between Nocturnal Sleep Duration and Insomnia symptoms with depressive symptoms among 44,900 Chinese Han adults aged 30-79 in Southwest China. BMC Psychiatry 23: 127.
  28. Lemke T, Hokby S, Wasserman D, Carli V, Hadlaczky G (2023) Associations between sleep habits, quality, chronotype and depression in a large cross-sectional sample of Swedish adolescents. PLoS One 18: e0293580.
  29. Lin S, Gong Q, Chen J, Wang J, Gao H, et al. (2023) Sleep duration is associated with depressive symptoms in Chinese adolescents. J Affect Disord 340: 64-70.
  30. Luo Y, Li Y, Xie J, Duan Y, Gan G, et al. (2022) Symptoms of depression are related to sedentary behavior and sleep duration in elderly individuals: A cross-sectional study of 49,317 older Chinese adults. J Affect Disord 308: 407-412.
  31. Ouyang P, Sun W (2019) Depression and sleep duration: findings from middle-aged and elderly people in China. Public Health 166: 148-154.
  32. Sakamoto N, Nanri A, Kochi T, Tsuruoka H, Pham NM, et al. (2013) Bedtime and sleep duration in relation to depressive symptoms among Japanese workers. J Occup Health 55: 479-486.
  33. Wang S, Rossheim ME, Nandy RR, Nguyen US (2024) Interaction between sleep duration and trouble sleeping on depressive symptoms among U.S. adults, NHANES 2015-2018. J Affect Disord 351: 285-292.
  34. Wu F, Chen SR, LI Y (2023) Correlation between sleep time and depression in middle-aged and elderly people in China. Journal of Hubei Medical University 42: 551-554.
  35. Yong Z, Wang K, Bai W, Li Y, Wu M, et al. (2024) Different correlation between depressive symptoms and sleep duration on weekdays/weekends among adolescents in Taiyuan, North China. J Affect Disord 358: 79-88.
  36. Zhang X, Li G, Shi C, Sun Y (2023) Associations of sleep duration, daytime napping, and snoring with depression in rural China: A cross-sectional study. BMC public health 23: 1530.
  37. Zhang XF, Liu F, Liu WP, Ye XM, Cui BY, et al. (2021) Relationship between sleep duration and depressive symptoms in middle-aged and elderly people in four provinces of China. Zhonghua Liu Xing Bing Xue Za Zhi 42: 1955-1961.
  38. Zhou T, Li R, Shi Y, Tian G, Yan Y (2022) The associations between sleep duration, cognitive function, and depressive symptoms: An analysis of Chinese adolescents from China Family Panel Studies. J Affect Disord 319: 252-259.
  39. Chang WW, Zhou MJ, Chen C, Hong Y, Zou X, et al. (2022) Correlation between sleep-wake circadian rhythm and anxiety, depression among medical students in Wuhu in 2020. Health Research 51: 417-422.
  40. Kim KM, Han SM, Heo K, Kim WJ, Chu MK (2020) Sex differences in the association between chronotype and risk of depression. Sci Rep 10: 18512.
  41. Kim SY, Kim HJ, Cho SS, Park MY, Kang MY (2023) Mediation analysis of chronotype, sleep-related factors, and depressive symptoms among workers: A cross- sectional study. Ann Occup Environ Med 35: e47.
  42. Koo DL, Yang KI, Kim JH, Kim D, Sunwoo JS, et al. (2021) Association between morningness-eveningness, sleep duration, weekend catch-up sleep and depression among Korean high-school students. J Sleep Res 30: e13063.
  43. Li T, Xie Y, Tao S, Yang Y, Xu H, et al. (2020) Chronotype, sleep, and depressive symptoms among chinese college students: A cross-sectional study. Front Neurol 11: 592825.
  44. Lv QY, Tang WJ, Wang Q, Deng W, Guo WJ, et al. (2021) Longitudinal study on the correlation between circadian rhythm type and depressive mood in college students. Journal of Chongqing Medical University 46: 1163-1169.
  45. Mao B, Xie Z, Liu M, Gong Y, Wang H, et al. (2024) Associations of chronotype with anxiety, depression and insomnia among general adult population: A cross-sectional study in Hubei, China. J Affect Disord 351: 250-258.
  46. Merikanto I, Kronholm E, Peltonen M, Laatikainen T, Vartiainen E, et al. (2015) Circadian preference links to depression in general adult population. J Affect Disord 188: 143-148.
  47. Norbury R (2019) Chronotype, depression and hippocampal volume: cross-sectional associations from the UK Biobank. Chronobiol Int 36: 709-716.
  48. Qu Y, Li T, Xie Y, Tao S, Yang Y, et al. (2023) Association of chronotype, social jetlag, sleep duration and depressive symptoms in Chinese college students. J Affect Disord 320: 735-741.
  49. Randler C, Stadler L, Vollmer C, Diaz-Morales JF (2012) Relationship between depressive symptoms and sleep duration/chronotype in women. Journal of Individual Differences 33: 186-191.
  50. Zhou J, Hsiao FC, Shi X, Yang J, Huang Y, et al. (2021) Chronotype and depressive symptoms: A moderated mediation model of sleep quality and resilience in the 1st-year college students. J Clin Psychol 77: 340-355.
  51. Islam Z, Hu H, Akter S, Kuwahara K, Kochi T, et al. (2020a) Social jetlag is associated with an increased likelihood of having depressive symptoms among the Japanese working population: the Furukawa Nutrition and Health Study. Sleep 43.
  52. Tamura N, Okamura K (2023) Social jetlag as a predictor of depressive symptoms among Japanese adolescents: Evidence from the Adolescent Sleep Health Epidemiological Cohort. Sleep Health 9: 638-644.
  53. Zhang YF, Fan YE, Ma ZJ, Wang DF, Fan F (2023) Associations of social jetlag and insomnia symptoms with depressive symptoms among Chinese adolescents: A large population-based study. Sleep Med 104: 98-104.
  54. van Dalfsen JH, Markus CR (2018) The influence of sleep on human hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis reactivity: A systematic review. Sleep Med Rev 39: 187-194.
  55. Walker MP, van der Helm E (2009) Overnight therapy? The role of sleep in emotional brain processing. Psychol Bull 135: 731-748.
  56. Ouyang M, Chen T, Chen J, Liu C, Luo H, et al. (2024) The association between sleep duration, bedtime, and cognitive ability in Chinese adults: Evidence from the China family panel studies. Heliyon 10: e30009.
  57. Bedrosian TA, Nelson RJ (2017) Timing of light exposure affects mood and brain circuits. Transl Psychiatry 7: e1017.
  58. Roenneberg T, Allebrandt KV, Merrow M, Vetter C (2012) Social jetlag and obesity. Curr biol 22: 939-943.
  59. Hasler BP, Buysse DJ, Kupfer DJ, Germain A (2010) Phase relationships between core body temperature, melatonin, and sleep are associated with depression severity: Further evidence for circadian misalignment in non-seasonal depression. Psychiatry Res 178: 205-207.
  60. Levandovski R, Dantas G, Fernandes LC, Caumo W, Torres I, et al. (2011) Depression scores associate with chronotype and social jetlag in a rural population. Chronobiol Int 28: 771-778.

Citation

Gao L, Gao J (2025) The Relationship of Sleep Duration, Chronotype, Social Jet Lag and the Risk of Depression: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Int J Depress Anxiety 8:043. doi.org/10.23937/2643-4059/1710043