Join Us | Latest Articles | Contact

Journal Home


Editorial Board


Archive


Submit to this journal


Current issue

International Journal of Sports and Exercise Medicine





DOI: 10.23937/2469-5718/1510053



Does Intensive Aerobic Training Influences Cognition in Middle-Aged Men?

Theodore Parthimos1*, Nikolaos Smyrnis2, Sokratis G Pappageorgiou3 and Ioannis Zalonis1


1Department of Neurology, Aeginition University Hospital, National & Kapodistrian University of Athens, Greece
2Department of Psychiatry, Aeginition University Hospital, National & Kapodistrian University of Athens, University Mental Health Research Institute of Athens, Greece
3Department of Neurology, Attikon University General Hospital, National & Kapodistrian University of Athens, Greece


*Corresponding author: Theodore Parthimos, Neuropsychological Laboratory, Department of Neurology, Aeginition University Hospital, Medical School, National & Kapodistrian University of Athens, 27-29 Chrysippou, 116 31, Athens, Greece, Tel: +302107622625, E-mail: thprth@med.uoa.gr, parththeo@yahoo.gr
Int J Sports Exerc Med, IJSEM-3-053, (Volume 3, Issue 2), Research Article; ISSN: 2469-5718
Received: December 16, 2016 | Accepted: March 08, 2017 | Published: March 10, 2017
Citation: Parthimos T, Smyrnis N, Pappageorgiou SG, Zalonis I (2017) Does Intensive Aerobic Training Influences Cognition in Middle-Aged Men? Int J Sports Exerc Med 3:053. 10.23937/2469-5718/1510053
Copyright: © 2017 Parthimos T, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.



Abstract

Purpose: The effect of chronic aerobic exercise on cognitive functions in middle-aged healthy adults.

Method: Thirty-three amateur runners (group of athletes) compared with thirty healthy adults with no previous involvement in sports (control group). Both groups were evaluated by means of comprehensive neuropsychological assessment for attention, processing speed, memory, visuospatial organization, executive functions and athletic test for aerobic capacity.

Results: The group of athletes had better performance in tests of scanning speed of visual information, in visuospatial-perceptual organization of new and complex information and shift and switching of attention.

Conclusion: Long-term aerobic exercise may be beneficial for cognitive functioning.


Keywords

Aerobic exercise, Cognitive function, Neuropsychological performance


Introduction

Normal aging is associated with thinning of both cortical and subcortical areas that lead to decline in cognitive process supported by these affected areas [1]. The effects of aging are especially pronounced within the domains of memory and executive functions [1], deficits of whose, particularly when they escalate to dementia, can have a dramatic impact on the independence, functionality and especially on patient's quality of life [2].

Physical exercise and sports have beneficial effects on the human body. Recent studies support that physical exercise may be a protective factor against ischemic stroke [3], and many other degenerative neurological diseases including Parkinson's [4] and Alzheimer's disease [5]. It is argued that 30 minutes or more exercise (3-5 days/week, 60-75% VO2) has a positive influence on cognitive function in healthy adults [6,7].

Aerobic exercise affects the density of cortical areas of the frontal and parietal lobes [8], which are highly involved in the speed of information processing and the effectiveness of executive functions [9]. Furthermore, the elderly with better aerobic capacity (compared to inactive but still normal elderly) have significantly less gray matter loss in the frontal, parietal and temporal lobes, less white matter loss in the anterior and posterior white matter tracts [10] and increased cerebral blood volume [11]. Additionally, they exhibit less decline with regards to verbal encoding and recall [12,13] and have increased volume of the anterior region of the hippocampus (including the dentate gyrus and area CA1) which is associated with improved spatial memory [14].

The beneficial effect of aerobic exercise has been demonstrated in animal studies as well. The structural changes in the brain of mice are associated with molecular and neurochemical changes, for example an increase of neurotrophic factors such as brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) that is observed on both young and older subjects after exercise [15]. These changes include dendritic sprouting, angiogenesis and growth of new neurons from stem cells with subsequent reinforcing processes that support learning and memory [15]. It is suggested that exercise helps to increase cell survival, proliferation and neurogenesis in the hippocampus resulting in improved spatial learning ability and long-term potentiation (LTP) or synaptic plasticity [16].

Aerobic exercise has linked with beneficial effect on specific cognitive functions. Guiney and Machado [17] refer that there are strong evidence of exercise-linked benefits related to task switching, selective attention, inhibition and working memory. Erickson, et al. [14] showed that aerobic exercise associated with improvements in spatial memory. Further, according to Smith, et al. [18] aerobic exercise training is associated with modest improvements in attention and processing speed, executive function and memory. Also, studies in middle-aged subjects report that participation in physical activity, even light, can significantly slow the rate of cognitive decline during later adulthood [19-21] or reduce the risk of developing dementia or Alzheimer's disease [22-24]. Identification of risk factors associated with cognitive decline, particularly earlier in the life course, is crucial in developing prevention or intervention strategies.

Given that physical, and particularly aerobic, exercise improves specific cognitive processes and that poor cognitive performance in middle age may be clinically relevant since individuals with mild cognitive impairment may progress to clinically diagnosed dementia at an accelerated rate (10-17% & 4.5-10% transition rate per year for specialized diagnostic centers and community centers, respectively) [25,26], the aim of the present study was to examine the effect of physical exercise on specific cognitive functions in middle-aged adults. Specifically, we hypothesized that physically active individuals would have better cognitive performance compared to non-active controls. We also investigated whether the degree of aerobic capacity would be associated with the participant's neuropsychological performance.


Methods

Subjects

Sixty-three middle-aged male volunteers were recruited from the community (age range: 45-55 years, education range: 9-16 years). Thirty-three amateur middle distance runners formed the group of athletes (experimental group). The group of athletes, according to their training background, involved in regular aerobic training routines such as running, cycling and competitive sports, (3-5 sessions per week with average duration one hour) since their adultness. All athletes, for at least the last five years, involved in regular running trainings (3-5 intense sessions per week with average duration one hour).

The other thirty subjects, who were not involved in sports, formed the control group. The control group did not take part in recreational physical activities such as walking and biking and none of the subjects had any background in regular aerobic training or competitive sports of any kind since adultness. Both groups were matched for age (age: 50.09 ± 2.95 vs. 50.20 ± 3.11 ( t29 = -0.17) and education education: 14.06 ± 2.00 vs. 14.00 ± 2.03 (t29 = 0.22). All participants were screened to exclude individuals with health problems (stroke, cardiovascular disease, neurological and psychiatric diseases, metabolic diseases and those receiving specific medication), as well as smokers and obese participants. All participants gave their written informed consent after having been informed about the purpose and procedures of the study. To exclude the effect of depressive and anxiety status, both groups were assessed using Beck Depression (BDI) and Inventory Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (SSTAI) [27,28].


Exercise and neuropsychological procedures

Exercise measures: All participants evaluated with Cooper 12 minute test [29-31] in order to define the fitness level. Subsequently, as the two groups differ significant in aerobic capacity level in Cooper 12 minutes test, in order to ensure that this difference is not due to exercise measuring procedure in which athletes are more familiar with, all participants ran or walked on a treadmill for forty minutes while their heart rate was monitored (training simulation). Participants were encouraged to run at the appropriate range heart rate for aerobic training (60-75%) [14]. Finally, anthropometric characteristics were measured before athletic test and compared across the two groups.

Neuropsychological testing: All participants were administered a series of neuropsychological tests to assess: a) attention and speed of information processing (Trail Making Test part A [TMT-A] [32], Symbol Digit Modalities Test [SDMT] [33]) b) visuospatial/perceptual ability (Rey's Complex Figure Test-copy form [RCFT-C]) [34]) c) verbal and visual episodic memory (Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test [RAVLT] [35], Rey's Complex Figure Test -recall forms [RCFT] [34], Babcock Story Recall Test [BSRT] [36]), and d) the effectiveness of executive functions (Trail Making Test part B [TMT-B] [32], Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test [SNST] [37]).

Neuropsychological procedures scoring: TMT-A score calculated in seconds by the completion of connection of a sequence of 25 consecutive targets on a sheet of paper. TMT-B score calculated in seconds by the completion of alternative connection between numbers and letters [32]. SDMT score, calculated by the number of correct symbols within the allowed time (90 sec) [33]. RCFT-C is scored for the accurate reproduction and placement of 18 specific design elements by copying. RCFT-IR and RCFT-DL are scored for the accurate reproduction and placement of 18 specific design elements from memory [34]. RAVLT-Tr 1-5 score calculated by the total number of words which are recalled in five presentations of a 15 word list. RAVLT-IR score calculated by the number of words which are immediately recalled after the intervention of a second word list. RAVLT-DL score calculated by the number of words which are recalled after a 20-30 minute delay [35]. BSRT-IR score calculated by elements of story which are recalled immediately. BSRT-DL score calculated by elements of story which are recalled after 20 minute delay [36]. SNST task score, calculated by subtracting the number of errors from the total number of items completed in 120 sec [37].


Statistical analysis

Before statistical analysis all variables were tested for normality using the Kolmogorov - Smirnov criterion. Numerical data are expressed as mean ± SD. T tests were used to compare mean values between groups (athletes vs. non-athletes). Pearson correlation analysis was applied to investigate any association between aerobic capacity and neuropsychological processes in both groups. SPSS 20.0 statistical package (SPSS, Chicago, IL) was used for statistical analysis. The level of statistical significance for the comparison of anthropometric characteristics as well as psychometric variables was Bonferoni corrected at P < 0.016 (0.05/3) and the level of statistical significance for the neuropsychological assessment was Bonferoni corrected at P < 0.004 (0.05/12).


Results

Table 1 presents the anthropometric characteristics for athlete and control groups. The athletes were taller than controls but this difference did not exceed the corrected level of significance for multiple comparisons. Also the athletes had a significantly smaller Body Mass Index (BMI) than controls.



Table 1: Anthropometric characteristics of athletes and non-athletes. View Table 1


Table 2 presents psychometric and neuropsychological performance measures of athletes and non-athletes. As seen in this table there were differences in specific neuropsychological performance measures between athletes and non-athletes while there was no difference in psychometric measures of depression and anxiety. More specifically athletes were faster than non-athletes both at TMT-A and TMT-B but the difference remained significant after correction for multiple comparisons only for TMT-B. Also, athletes performed significantly better than non-athletes at the RCFT copy neuropsychological test.



Table 2: Between-group differences (athletes vs. non-athletes) on depression, anxiety and neuropsychological performance. View Table 2


In addition, in Cooper 12 minutes test the group of athletes performed at excellent level t (29) = 39.15, p = 0.001, (2668 ± 149.19 m/12 min) whereas the group of non-athletes performed at below average level (1437 ± 86.07 m/12min). In training simulation we also found significant differences t (29) = 34.52, p = 0.001, on aerobic capacity between athletes (8.13 ± 0.47 km/40 min) and non-athletes (5.12 ± 0.40), as expected.


Discussion

The results of this study are in agreement with previous research concerning the effects of physical exercise on cognitive functions. The two groups differed in TMT-A with athletes performing better compared to non-athletes although this difference was not significant for multiple comparisons. This finding is suggestive of differences in attentional performance and especially in visuomotor tracking [10]. A 10 week regular exercise training in healthy participants improved performance on executive function tasks, including TMT [38]. Also, athletes showed significantly better performance in copying the complex shape of the Rey's CFT. Copying this complex shape besides perceptual processing, requires effective range of working memory, cognitive adoption strategy, planning, coordination and monitoring of operations until the completion of the project [39]. Brown, et al. [40] observed significant positive associations between physical exercise and the copy form of the RCFT. Furthermore, West [41] suggested that these tasks require constant mediation by a center executor because they do not become automatic and therefore the benefits of exercise would be reflected in these executive processes. It has been also suggested that physical exercise effects might be most readily observed on visuospatial tasks because visuospatial processes have been demonstrated to be more susceptible to aging than verbal skills [42,43].

Although it has been found that aerobic exercise has positive effects on memory function [21,44,45], in the present study we did not found any significant differences in neuropsychological measures of memory function between athletes and controls.

Also, our groups differed significantly in TMT-B with athletes performing better than non-athletes. These finding suggest differences in components of executive function, i.e. the switching and shift of attention and cognitive flexibility. Previous studies have confirmed the beneficial effects of aerobic exercise on cognitive functions supported by the frontal and prefrontal cortex [46]. Liu-Ambrose, et al. [47] refer that an exercise program can enhance the executive cognitive function of selective attention and conflict resolution in elderly. Moreover, acute exercise improves performance on executive cognitive tasks such as planning, inhibition and working memory [10,48]. Our results are also in agreement with significantly improved cognitive flexibility in moderate and high exercise groups compared with control (non-exercise) group [49].

According to Singh-Manoux, et al. [19] physical activity moderates the decline in cognitive functioning typically associated with aging. The aspect of cognitive functioning that declines most with age is fluid intelligence [50], (e.g. visuomotor scanning-attention, visuoperceptual organization skill, set shifting, cognitive flexibility) which seems to be intrinsically associated with information processing and involves short-term memory, abstract thinking, creativity, ability to solve novel problems, and reaction time. This aspect is also the one the benefits the most from long term exercise as shown in this study.

The observed differences between the two groups, although it does not indicate any pathology in anyway, however support the suggestion of the beneficial effect of long-term aerobic exercise on cognitive functions also in middle-aged healthy population and confirm its importance in successful aging.

Although several viable hypotheses have been proposed, the mechanisms underlying the association between physical activity and cognitive functioning are poorly understood. It is important to theorize possible explanations for our findings to further support future research in this field. It may be that physical exercise benefits cognitive function through disease reduction (e.g. Cardiovascular disease, Stroke, Diabetes, Hypertension) and/or enhance brain structure and function (e.g. Increase production and efficiency of neurotransmitters, Angiogenesis, Synaptogenesis, Neurogenesis) [41].

There were specific limitations in this study. First, the relatively small sample size is a limiting factor. Second, the group of athletes included only people with high aerobic capacity. The aerobic capacity depends on the training session frequency and intensity. In this study, athletes had a long and systematic sport engagement and for that reason it is likely that their aerobic capacity approached the upper limit of normal. This fact could result to an apparent ceiling effect, canceling the effect of correlation with any modulating variable. Also the use of various training programs, different time lags and reevaluation should be tested to better understand the residual effects of aerobic exercise. Furthermore, many of aging effects are first noted at 60 years of age or older, and therefore do not relate to this middle-aged cohort. Finally, the study subjects were only men and we cannot conclude whether our findings would apply to women.

In conclusion, the findings indicate that physical activity is an important factor in cognitive functioning in middle age and that its effects appear earlier than previously reported. It should be noted that the components of cognition which further deteriorate over the years are those of attention and speed of information processing and the effectiveness of executive functions, and our results suggest that physical activity, specifically long-term aerobic exercise, may be beneficial for these same cognitive functions and reduce the risk of early cognitive decline in middle age.


Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the participants for the willingness to participate in the present study.


References
  1. Hertzog C, Kramer AF, Wilson RS, Lindenberger U (2009) Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: Can the functional capacity of older adults be Preserved and Enhanced? Psychol Sci Public Interest 9: 1-65.

  2. Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Head D, Kennedy KM, Acker JD (2004) Differential aging of the medial temporal lobe: a study of a five-year change. Neurology 62: 433-438.

  3. Stummer W, Weber K, Tranmer B, Baethmann A, Kempski O (1994) Reduced mortality and brain damage after locomotor activity in gerbil forebrain ischemia. Stroke 25: 1862-1869.

  4. Smith AD, Zigmond MJ (2003) Can the brain be protected through exercise? Lessons from an animal model of parkinsonism. Exp Neurol 184: 31-39.

  5. Cotman CW, Berchtold NC (2002) Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends Neurosci 25: 295-301.

  6. Chang YK, Labban JD, Gapin JI, Etnier JL (2012) The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Brain Res 1453: 87-101.

  7. McAuley E, Kramer AF, Colcombe SJ (2004) Cardiovascular fitness and neurocognitive function in older adults: a brief review. Brain Behav Immun 18: 214-220.

  8. van Boxtel MP, Paas FG, Houx PJ, Adam JJ, Teeken JC, et al. (1997) Aerobic capacity and cognitive performance in a cross-sectional aging study. Med Sci Sports Exerc 29: 1357-1365.

  9. Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Raz N, Webb AG, Cohen NJ, et al. (2003) Aerobic fitness reduces brain tissue loss in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 58: 176-180.

  10. Kramer AF, Hahn S, Cohen NJ, Banich MT, McAuley E, et al. (1999) Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature 400: 418-419.

  11. Rogers RL, Meyer JS, Mortel KF (1990) After reaching retirement age physical activity sustains cerebral perfusion and cognition. J Am Geriatr Soc 38: 123-128.

  12. Miller KJ, Siddarth P, Gaines JM, Parrish JM, Ercoli LM, et al. (2012) The memory fitness program: cognitive effects of a healthy aging intervention. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 20: 514-523.

  13. Stewart R, Prince M, Mann A (2003) Age, vascular risk, and cognitive decline in an older, British, African-Caribbean population. J Am Geriatr Soc 51: 1547-1553.

  14. Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, et al. (2011) Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108: 3017-3022.

  15. Rosenzweig MR, Bennett EL (1996) Psychobiology of plasticity: effects of training and experience on brain and behavior. Behav Brain Res 78: 57-65.

  16. van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH (1999) Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus. Nat Neurosci 2: 266-270.

  17. Guiney H, Machado L, Guiney H, Machado L (2012) Benefits of regular aerobic exercise for executive functioning in healthy populations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 20: 73-86.

  18. Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA, Hoffman BM, Cooper H, Strauman TA, et al. (2010) Aerobic Exercise and Neurocognitive Performance: A Meta-Analytic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Psychosom Med 72: 239-252.

  19. Singh-Manoux A, Hillsdon M, Brunner E, Marmot M (2005) Effects of physical activity on cognitive functioning in middle age: evidence from the Whitehall II prospective cohort study. Am J Public Health 95: 2252-2258.

  20. Richards M, Hardy R, Wadsworth ME (2003) Does active leisure protect cognition? Evidence from a national birth cohort. Soc Sci Med 56: 785-792.

  21. Bosma H, van Boxtel MP, Ponds RW, Jelicic M, Houx P, et al. (2002) Engaged lifestyle and cognitive function in middle and old-aged, non-demented persons: a reciprocal association? Z Gerontol Geriatr 35: 575-581.

  22. Geda YE, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, Christianson TJ, Pankratz VS, et al. (2010) Physical exercise, aging, and mild cognitive impairment: a population-based study. Arch Neurol 67: 80-86.

  23. Andel R, Crowe M, Pedersen NL, Fratiglioni L, Johansson B, et al. (2008) Physical exercise at midlife and risk of dementia three decades later: a population-based study of Swedish twins. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 63: 62-66.

  24. Rovio S, Kåreholt I, Helkala EL, Viitanen M, Winblad B, et al. (2005) Leisure-time physical activity at midlife and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol 4: 705-711.

  25. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, et al. (1999) Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 56: 303-308.

  26. Petersen RC, Negash S (2008) Mild cognitive impairment: an overview. CNS Spectr 13: 45-53.

  27. Zalonis I, Christidi F, Kolovou D, Pantes G, Sgouropoulos P, et al. (2008) Assesssing episodic memory: Normative data and discriminant properties for a Greek version of Babcock Story Recall Test. Athens: Paper presented at the Third Dual Congress of Psychiatry and Neurosciences.

  28. Zalonis I, Christidi, Bonakis A, Kararizou E, Triantafyllou NI, et al. (2009) The stroop effect in Greek healthy population: normative data for the Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 24: 81-88.

  29. Cooper KH (1968) A means of assessing maximal oxygen intake. Correlation between field and treadmill testing. JAMA 203: 201-204.

  30. Grant S, Corbett K, Amjad AM, Wilson J, Aitchison T (1995) A comparison of methods of predicting maximum oxygen uptake. Br J Sports Med 29: 147-152.

  31. Zwiren LD, Freedson PS, Ward A, Wilke S, Rippe JM (1991) Estimation of VO2max: a comparative analysis of five exercise tests. Res Q Exerc Sport 62: 73-78.

  32. Zalonis I, Kararizou E, Triantafyllou NI, Kapaki E, Papageorgiou S, et al. (2008) A normative study of the trail making test A and B in Greek adults. Clin Neuropsychol 22: 842-850.

  33. Smith A (1982) Symbol Digit Modalities Test Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

  34. Meyers J, Meyers K (1995) Rey Complex Figure and Recognition Trial: Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

  35. Messinis L, Tsakona I, Malefaki S, Papathanasopoulos P (2007) Normative data and discriminant validity of Rey's Verbal Learning Test for the Greek adult population. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 22: 739-752.

  36. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J (1961) An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 4: 561-571.

  37. Spielberger CD, Gorssuch RL, Lushene PR, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA (1983) Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.

  38. Young RJ (1979) The effect of regular exercise on cognitive functioning and personality. Br J Sports Med 13: 110-117.

  39. Waber DP, Holmes JM (1986) Assessing children's memory productions of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 8: 563-580.

  40. Brown BM, Peiffer JJ, Sohrabi HR, Mondal A, Gupta VB, et al. (2012) Intense physical activity is associated with cognitive performance in the elderly. Transl Psychiatry 2: 191.

  41. West RL (1996) An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychol Bull 120: 272-292.

  42. Shay KA, Roth DL (1992) Association between aerobic fitness and visuospatial performance in healthy older adults. Psychol Aging 7: 15-24.

  43. Stones MJ, Kozma A (1989) Age, exercise, and coding performance. Psychol Aging 4: 190-194.

  44. Stroth S, Hille K, Spitzer M, Reinhardt R (2009) Aerobic endurance exercise benefits memory and affect in young adults. Neuropsychol Rehabil 19: 223-243.

  45. Hötting K, Schauenburg G, Röder B (2012) Long-term effects of physical exercise on verbal learning and memory in middle-aged adults: results of a one-year follow-up study. Brain Sci 2: 332-346.

  46. Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, Kim JS, Prakash R, et al. (2006) Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 61: 1166-1170.

  47. Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Graf P, Beattie BL, Ashe MC, et al. (2010) Resistance training and executive functions: a 12-month randomized controlled trial. Arch Intern Med 170: 170-178.

  48. Tsung-Min Hung, Chia-Liang Tsai, Feng-Tzu Chen, Chun-Chih Wang, Yu-Kai Chang (2013) The immediate and sustained effects of acute exercise on planning aspect of executive function. Psychology of Sports and Exercise 14: 728-736.

  49. Masley S, Roetzheim R, Gualtieri T (2009) Aerobic exercise enhances cognitive flexibility. J Clin Psychol Med Settings 16: 186-193.

  50. Cattell RB (1963) Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: a critical experiment. J Educ Psychol 54: 1-22.

Go to top
International Journal of Anesthetics and Anesthesiology (ISSN: 2377-4630)
International Journal of Blood Research and Disorders   (ISSN: 2469-5696)
International Journal of Brain Disorders and Treatment (ISSN: 2469-5866)
International Journal of Cancer and Clinical Research (ISSN: 2378-3419)
International Journal of Clinical Cardiology (ISSN: 2469-5696)
Journal of Clinical Gastroenterology and Treatment (ISSN: 2469-584X)
Clinical Medical Reviews and Case Reports (ISSN: 2378-3656)
Journal of Dermatology Research and Therapy (ISSN: 2469-5750)
International Journal of Diabetes and Clinical Research (ISSN: 2377-3634)
Journal of Family Medicine and Disease Prevention (ISSN: 2469-5793)
Journal of Genetics and Genome Research (ISSN: 2378-3648)
Journal of Geriatric Medicine and Gerontology (ISSN: 2469-5858)
International Journal of Immunology and Immunotherapy (ISSN: 2378-3672)
International Journal of Medical Nano Research (ISSN: 2378-3664)
International Journal of Neurology and Neurotherapy (ISSN: 2378-3001)
International Archives of Nursing and Health Care (ISSN: 2469-5823)
International Journal of Ophthalmology and Clinical Research (ISSN: 2378-346X)
International Journal of Oral and Dental Health (ISSN: 2469-5734)
International Journal of Pathology and Clinical Research (ISSN: 2469-5807)
International Journal of Pediatric Research (ISSN: 2469-5769)
International Journal of Respiratory and Pulmonary Medicine (ISSN: 2378-3516)
Journal of Rheumatic Diseases and Treatment (ISSN: 2469-5726)
International Journal of Sports and Exercise Medicine (ISSN: 2469-5718)
International Journal of Stem Cell Research & Therapy (ISSN: 2469-570X)
International Journal of Surgery Research and Practice (ISSN: 2378-3397)
Trauma Cases and Reviews (ISSN: 2469-5777)
International Archives of Urology and Complications (ISSN: 2469-5742)
International Journal of Virology and AIDS (ISSN: 2469-567X)
More Journals

Contact Us

ClinMed International Library | Science Resource Online LLC
3511 Silverside Road, Suite 105, Wilmington, DE 19810, USA
Email: contact@clinmedlib.org
 

Feedback

Get Email alerts
 
Creative Commons License
Open Access
by ClinMed International Library is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License based on a work at https://clinmedjournals.org/.
Copyright © 2017 ClinMed International Library. All Rights Reserved.